Paper - The status of metamerism in the central nervous system of chick embryos: Difference between revisions

From Embryology
mNo edit summary
mNo edit summary
Line 24: Line 24:
Six Figures (1933)
Six Figures (1933)


In the laboratory in which these lines are being written
==Introduction==
there has been installed recently a memorial exhibit in commemoration of Wilhelm His.‘ It was His who first called
In the laboratory in which these lines are being written there has been installed recently a memorial exhibit in commemoration of Wilhelm His.‘ It was His who first called attention to the importance of the establishment of an institute for human embryology and who formulated the general plan upon which such an institute should be conducted. It was left for Mall, a devoted apostle, to make the His dream a reality and to guide its organization as it came into existence under the form of the Department of Embryology of the Carnegie Institution of Washington. It is left to us and succeeding generations to pause before this exhibit as a shrine and contemplate the contributions of a great scientist and to rejoice in the fine friendship that existed between him a.nd one of our own great men.
attention to the importance of the establishment of an institute for human embryology and who formulated the general
plan upon which such an institute should be conducted. It
was left for Mall, a devoted apostle, to make the His dream
a reality and to guide its organization as it came into existence under the form of the Department of Embryology of
the Carnegie Institution of Washington. It is left to us and
succeeding generations to pause before this exhibit as a shrine
and contemplate the contributions of a great scientist and to
rejoice in the fine friendship that existed between him a.nd one
of our own great men.


‘ The His historical exhibit was made possible through Prof. W. His, his son,
who had in his possession the majority of the items and who has deposited them
in the Carnegie Laboratory for the purposes of the memorial. There are included
a large number of personal copies of his monographs and separates containing
marginal notes and sketches; original manuscript of his important paper “Héiute
und Hiihlen des Korpers”; lecture notebook, recorded by W. Braunc, in which
are his famous lecture—drawings; microscope and lenses used by Professor His
from 1856 to 1904; ombryograph invented by him and with which much of his
work was done, the predecessor of the camera lucida; a. box of photographic
transparencies made by him and on which the illustrations in the “Atlas of
human embryos” were based; comparative embryological collection of more than
1000 slides prepared by him, covering a wide range of animal forms and the


original material on which many of his researches were made; various portraitphotographs and a copy of the Hans Olde etching.
* The His historical exhibit was made possible through Prof. W. His, his son, who had in his possession the majority of the items and who has deposited them in the Carnegie Laboratory for the purposes of the memorial. There are included a large number of personal copies of his monographs and separates containing marginal notes and sketches; original manuscript of his important paper “Héiute und Hiihlen des Korpers”; lecture notebook, recorded by W. Braunc, in which are his famous lecture—drawings; microscope and lenses used by Professor His from 1856 to 1904; ombryograph invented by him and with which much of his work was done, the predecessor of the camera lucida; a box of photographic transparencies made by him and on which the illustrations in the “Atlas of human embryos” were based; comparative embryological collection of more than 1000 slides prepared by him, covering a wide range of animal forms and the original material on which many of his researches were made; various portraitphotographs and a copy of the Hans Olde etching.


455
456 G. L. STREETER


One of the fields of investigation to which His made important eontributions extend over the same ground with which
One of the fields of investigation to which His made important eontributions extend over the same ground with which this paper is concerned, namely, the natural subdivisions of the neural tube. VVhile 11ot regarded as one of his major accomplishments, the subdivisions introduced by him in the “N omina anatomica” have come i11to universal use. From careful studies of the development of the human brain and the brain of embryos of other vertebrate classes, he arrived at subdivisions that are applicable to embryonic stages of the various animals as Well as to the adult brain. He provided the anatomist with a morphological analysis of the brain based on the earliest stages and determined the relations of the separate parts to one another and traced their subsequent increasing complexity of form. In this way he greatly simplified neural topography and advanced our understanding of a most intricate organ.
this paper is concerned, namely, the natural subdivisions of
the neural tube. VVhile 11ot regarded as one of his major accomplishments, the subdivisions introduced by him in the
“N omina anatomica” have come i11to universal use. From
careful studies of the development of the human brain and
the brain of embryos of other vertebrate classes, he arrived
at subdivisions that are applicable to embryonic stages of the
various animals as Well as to the adult brain. He provided
the anatomist with a morphological analysis of the brain
based on the earliest stages and determined the relations of
the separate parts to one another and traced their subsequent
increasing complexity of form. In this way he greatly simplified neural topography and advanced our understanding of
a most intricate organ.


During the time that His and the other members of his
 
committee, Krause and Waldeyer, were conservatively arriving at brain analysis and terminology based on demonstrable morphological realities, a different direction was taken
During the time that His and the other members of his committee, Krause and Waldeyer, were conservatively arriving at brain analysis and terminology based on demonstrable morphological realities, a different direction was taken by the comparative embryologists. Apparently intoxicated by the simplification of the mechanics of development that seemed to be offered by the principle of metamerism, they did not stop their metamerism with the middle germ layer, but were applying it also to the other tissues including the medullary tube. Several investigators in this country joined in this movement. Among them was Dr. Charles Hill, working in the Zoological Laboratory of Northwestern University and under the influence of Professor Locy? I mention Hill because of the wide use that has been made of his observations. His drawings of the early stages of the brain and his presentation of neural metamerism is in general harmony with the present prevailing views among teachers and writers in this field.
by the comparative embryologists. Apparently intoxicated
by the simplification of the mechanics of development that
seemed to be offered by the principle of metamerism, they
did not stop their metamerism with the middle germ layer, but
were applying it also to the other tissues including the medullary tube. Several investigators in this country joined in this
movement. Among them was Dr. Charles Hill, working in the
Zoological Laboratory of Northwestern University and under
the influence of Professor Locy? I mention Hill because of
the wide use that has been made of his observations. His
drawings of the early stages of the brain and his presentation
of neural metamerism is in general harmony with the present
prevailing views among teachers and writers in this field.


2 Hill, C. 1899. Primary segments of the vertebrate head. Anat. Anz., Bd. 16,
2 Hill, C. 1899. Primary segments of the vertebrate head. Anat. Anz., Bd. 16,


S. 353-369. Also, 1900, Developmental hitory of primary segments of the vertebrate head. Zool. Jahrb., Bd. 13, S. 393-446.
S. 353-369. Also, 1900, Developmental hitory of primary segments of the vertebrate head. Zool. Jahrb., Bd. 13, S. 393-446.  
METAMERISM IN CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM 457


In brief, the Hill-Locy interpretation of metamerism in
the embryonic brain holds that the encephalic portion of
the neural axis is divided into some eleven ‘similar joints
or segments.’ In the fore- and mid-brain these are transient, being soon obliterated and succeeded by secondary
modifications or expansions. In the hind-brain, however, the
primitive segments persist longer and thus for a time are contemporaneous and might be mistaken as being in series with
the secondary expansions of the fore- and mid-brain. It is
emphasized that they are not really coordinate with the latter,
but are identical with the evanescent segments which have
faded out in the more oral levels.


In his papers of 1899 and 1900, Hill published a series of
In brief, the Hill-Locy interpretation of metamerism in the embryonic brain holds that the encephalic portion of the neural axis is divided into some eleven ‘similar joints or segments.’ In the fore- and mid-brain these are transient, being soon obliterated and succeeded by secondary modifications or expansions. In the hind-brain, however, the primitive segments persist longer and thus for a time are contemporaneous and might be mistaken as being in series with the secondary expansions of the fore- and mid-brain. It is emphasized that they are not really coordinate with the latter, but are identical with the evanescent segments which have faded out in the more oral levels.
drawings of salmon and chick embryos in which neural segmentation as interpreted by him is shown with diagrammatic
clarity. Those of the chick are the ones that have been so
frequently reproduced in books on embryology, and as typical
examples I am herewith showing two of his drawings (fig. 1).
They portray the 4-somite stage, at which time he finds the
Whole gamut of segments in their maximum distinctness.
Since the principle of neural metamerism is so largely based
on this and similar evidence, it becomes important to make
certain of the morphological accuracy of the underlying observations. If the neural tube is actually segmented as Hill
pictures it, one must accept some such interpretation as he
and the other segmentalists have elaborated.


It is with this thought that the writer has gone back to the
In his papers of 1899 and 1900, Hill published a series of drawings of salmon and chick embryos in which neural segmentation as interpreted by him is shown with diagrammatic clarity. Those of the chick are the ones that have been so frequently reproduced in books on embryology, and as typical examples I am herewith showing two of his drawings (fig. 1). They portray the 4-somite stage, at which time he finds the Whole gamut of segments in their maximum distinctness. Since the principle of neural metamerism is so largely based on this and similar evidence, it becomes important to make certain of the morphological accuracy of the underlying observations. If the neural tube is actually segmented as Hill pictures it, one must accept some such interpretation as he and the other segmentalists have elaborated.
chick embryo and, through utilization of the available improvements in morphological technique, has sought to verify
 
the presence or absence of transitory metameric segmentation of the brain. The results of this study are reported in
It is with this thought that the writer has gone back to the chick embryo and, through utilization of the available improvements in morphological technique, has sought to verify the presence or absence of transitory metameric segmentation of the brain. The results of this study are reported in the following paper. In summarizing these the writer finds himself in full agreement with Froriep,"* who, 40 years ago, expressed the opinion that metameres of the medullary tube, in the sense of primitive members of a segmentally laid-down organ system, are not justified by the available evidence.
the following paper. In summarizing these the writer finds
himself in full agreement with Froriep,"* who, 40 years ago,
expressed the opinion that metameres of the medullary tube,
in the sense of primitive members of a segmentally laid-down
organ system, are not justified by the available evidence.


‘Froriep, A. 1892. Verhandl. Anat. Gesel1., Wien, Ergi1'.nzungsh., Anat. Anz.
‘Froriep, A. 1892. Verhandl. Anat. Gesel1., Wien, Ergi1'.nzungsh., Anat. Anz.
458 G. L. STREETER
Segmentation of the vertebrate body seems to be originally
limited to the middle germ—layer and Where segmental arrangement occurs in derivatives of the ectoderm it is of
secondary origin and is an adaptation to the metamerism of
the mesoblast.
Photographic records of the brain of the 4—sornite chick do
not reveal the segmented condition pictured by Hill (fig. 1),
 
:;-.a.-.;.;,,'.‘..
.4: 3-‘..'.<.é.o‘, .5 «z. .;.i..
fig.1 Brain segments in the 4—somito chick according to H.ill (1899). This
frequently copied figure illustrates neural segmentation with great daring. When
this is compared with photographs of actual specimens (fig. 2), scant justification is found for the segmentation as shown. Only the somite grooves are
found in the photographs.
nor do the next older or younger stages exhibit constrictions
that can be satisfactorily fitted in with such a system of uniform segmentation. Instead of definable metameres which
can be traced into specific brain parts, it is found that certain
of the functional subdivisions of the brain reveal their identity
Very early and before the closure of the neural folds. As
development progresses, new subdivisions and modifying
METAMERISM IN CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM 459
fig.2 Ventral and dorsal views showing tlm neural folds in four chick
embryos of about the 4-somite stage for c-,omp:1rison with figure 1. Flnlargmnmut,
X 20.
460 G. L. STREETER
mechanisms detach from these or appear in the intervals between them. To what extent these more tardy subdivisions
are predetermined structures and to what extent they are the
product of interaction between the pioneer mechanisms is
beyond the province of this paper. Morphologically, however,
the various nuclear masses as they successively make their
appearance and the communicating fiber-laminae can be
traced step by step as they individually grow in size and
complexity until the final form is attained. All of this takes
place in the absence of any true metameric segmentation.
Qualification should perhaps be made for the visceral cranial
nerves. That portion of the neural axis from which they arise
is characterized by marked transverse grooves, and if this
group of nerves is to be regarded as segmental then the
grooves produced by the proliferating neuroblasts giving
origin to the nerves must also be held as segmental. But
that is not granting much to neural metamerism.
CHICK BRAIN AT CLOSURE OF NEURAL TUBE
The chick is one of the most thoroughly studied of all embryonic types. It has been elaborately and systematically
portrayed in atlas form and, moreover, in a whole series of
atlases, beautiful and well known.‘ But even in this abundance of material it is difficult to determine whether I-Iill’s
evanescent segments of the brain are present or not. They
cannot be seen satisfactorily in total cleared preparations
studied by transmitted light, the method heretofore invariably
employed. In such specimens one has only incomplete
glimpses of surfaces, rendered uncertain by refraction and
overlapping of structures and inaccessible to special illumination. Consequently, a large personal equation enters in the
selection of significant levels and contours. Nor do serial
sections and reconstruction methods suffice. In face of these
difficulties, it was concluded that the desired information as
‘E1-dl, M. 1’. 1845. Die Entwiekelung dos Menschen und des Hiihnchens im
Eie. Leipzig.
Duval, M. .1899. Atlas d’embryologie. Paris.
Keibel, F. 12. K. Abraham. 1900. Normentafel zur Entwicklungsgeschichte des
I-Iulmes. Jena.
METAMERISM IN CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM 461
to the true form of the neural tube must be obtained from
specimens whose tissues are rendered opaque and can be
freely exposed to direct illumination by means of dissection
and under circumstances that introduce the minimum amount
of artifact. It was found that chick embryos having these
requirements could be prepared by fixing blastoderms in
Bouin’s solution which are then rinsed and dehydrated up
to 80 per cent alcohol. In the latter medium they are dissected in various ways and photographed with carefully adjusted illuminations. At the stages in which we are interested
the embryos are very tiny and magnification is necessary,
the original negatives being made at an enlargement of 12
diameters. In making prints for study the enlargement is
.further increased to 25 or 50 diameters. A series of such
photographs are shown in figure 2 and they will now be
described.
The four stages shown in figure 2 were selected as representing the same period in which Hill found such well-defined
brain segments and they should be closely compared with his
figure reproduced in figure 1. At this time the neural folds
are just beginning to close and there are from 3 to 4 separated
somites. A and A’ of figure 2 are dorsal and ventral views
of the youngest specimen in the group. The endoderm and
heart have been removed, exposing the ventral surface of
the primary sheet of mesoderm (A’). Hensen’s node can be
seen, and extending forward from it is the flat notochord.
Lateral to the notochord on each side are the stripsiof the
thickened mesoderm from which the somites are being segmented, three pairs can already be seen. The dorsal View
of the same specimen (A) shows the neural folds as still
rather flat, except at the front end where there is active
proliferation of the constituent cells and a relative excess of
tissue, particularly in theregion where the eye evagination
is to take place. The proliferation of cells occurs in such a
way that their margins are turned up and tend to approximate. In gross appearance it is as though the opposing
plates were pressed together with thumb and finger. The
run JOURNAL or 00MT“ARA'1‘I\'E NEUROLOGY, Von. 57, No. 3
462 G. L. STREETER
point to be noticed is that there is no distinguishable segmentation, except in the region of the mesodermal somites,
which appear to be the elements responsible for the phenomenon. There are slight irregularities elsewhere along
the margins of the folds, but they are less marked and bear
no more evidence of being segmental than those characterizing the margins of the primitive groove in this same
specimen.
The next specimen (fig. 2, B and B’) is slightly older. The
primitive pit is more distinct and at that point on the ventral
surface there is a prominent Hensen’s node, set apart from
t.he primitive streak caudal to it. The fourth pair of mesodermal somites ca11 be recognized. The notochord can be
seen lyi11g along the mid-line and taking its origin from
Hensen’s node. The neural plates lie flat as in the 3—somite
stage, except at the front end where they are bent upward
and their margins pressed against each other. In the ventral
view (B’) the neural axis is correspondingly narrower, although there is already a little‘ fullness where the optic
evagination is to take place. As in the previous specimen,
there is no evidence of orderly segmentation other than that
concerned with the somites. Nor is there so far, if we adhere
to the material evidence before us, anything in the way of a
brain vesicle.
The next two specimens (fig. 2, C and D) are essentially
representatives of the same stage as the last specimen, showing variants in form. In one of them (0') the optic evagination is more advanced than in B’ and in the other |D’) it is
less advanced. Otherwise the closing neural tube is smooth
and devoid of segmentation, excepting in the region of the
somites. That the mesodermal somi.tes influence the form
of the neural ectoderm with which they are in contact can be
seen by stripping off the mesoderm, as was done in D’. Here
one can speak at least of secondary segmentation. Whether
these neural segments would form in the absence of mesodermal somites must be determined before the question can
be answered as to their primary or secondary nature. ElseMETAMERISM IN CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM 463
where the neural plates during this period are devoid of any
such phenomenon. Nor do these photographs show as yet any
brain vesicles.
The outline drawings in figure 3 represent the initial phases
in the closure of the neural tube. As we shall see, separation
of the tube into subdivisions begins even as early as this and
the effort has been made to record the form of the tube as
accurately as possible. The contours were obtained by making tracings of enlarged photographs. Thus, the 3- and
4-somite stages of figure 3 are from the same specimens shown
in figure 2, A and B, with which they should be compared.
The others were made from similar photographs of various
other specimens. In all cases the drawings were controlled
by the original embryonic material under the binocular microscope. The drawings were all made at the same enlargement
so that one can follow any alterations in width or length.
For their final rendering in ink I am indebted to the expert
hand of Mr. J. F. I)idusch.
In comparing these stages the transformation of the caudal
part of the germ—disk can be dismissed with few words. This
is the zone of primary differentiation where, through the
activity of Hensen’s node and the contiguous primitive streak,
certain elements, such as mesoderm and notochord, are sepa~
rated out from the ectodermal caudal germ—bed and there is
left in the path behind, or rather in front, the median longitudinal chordal scar, 011 each side of which are the paired
11eura.l plates which are to become the central nervous system.
This constitutes the primary exodus of non-nervous elements
from the ectoderm. As growth proceeds, the zone where this
neural purification is taking place is found progressively
further and further back, remaining characteristically, to the
very last, flat and open just as was the original germ-disk.
. Confining our attention to the purified neural plates, it will
be seen that their closure into a tube begins in the chick, near
the eye region and is effected by the bending up of the plates
so that their margins are pressed together and the same
process spreads from this region caudalward simultaneously
Eye
464
Thalamus
o +ic
P lobe
\[Ll‘VIH
9’?
G. L. STREETER
 
35.
916 = closure inccmplefe
\ 0 \\
6~75. \/
55, 11-12 5.
fig.3 Outline dr:1wi11gs of’ the chick brain during the period of c011versi011 into 3. 11011172.! tube, showing
certain precocious primitive parts \vhic,h already 9:411 be recognized {it that time. These structures determine the
contours of the neural tube. It will be seen that lnetameric segmentation is present only in the region of the
mesodermal somites. The specimens are all enlarged 25 diameters.
METAMERISM IN CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM 465
with the elongation of the neural axis. At first the margins
merely press against each other. This is the condition up to
the 6- and 7-somite stages. At the 8-somite stage actual
fusion has taken place along the line where the first contact
occurred. From this point the phenomenon of closure or
fusion of the mid-dorsal seam spreads forward and caudalward and in doing so gives rise to the neural crest which will
not be dealt with here. That much will be generally granted.
But while the tube is closing there is active proliferation of
the cells composing its wall. This proliferation, like the
closure itself, occurs earlier at the oral end, spreading thence
backward, and, furthermore, it is more active in certain zones
resulting in a fullness or expansion of the corresponding
areas, characteristic for the stage and the species. It is the
significance of the consequent irregularities in contour of the
neural tube which I wish to raise in question. I cannot see
that these are orderly uniform metameric segments as pietured by Hill, instead they appear to be definite specialized
structures, the primordia of the more primitive mechanisms,
which can be followed through. into the completed brain.
As an example of a definitive part of the brain, whose primordium can be recognized very early in the embryo, the chick
offers a very conspicuous one in the visual apparatus. Already in the 3- and 4-somite stages there is a fullness in the
region where the eye is to appear. At the 6-somite stage, as
can be seen in figure 3, there is on each side a well-demarcated
evagination expanding laterally. From then on these bilateral evaginations are unmistakably the eyes. It is to be
noted that neither dorsally, anteriorly, nor ventrally do these
meet in the middle line. In other words, in the 6- to 7-somite
stage we are not dealing with a primary brain vesicle, but
with two eye vesicles separated by a part of the brain Wall
that is to form other important structures less far advanced.
Furthermore, the eye vesicles are there before the dorsal
margins actually come together and before one could postulate
a primary brain vesicle. Following closely after the formation of the eye vesicles are the optic thalami and the optic
466 G. L. STREETER
lobes which are also essentially bilateral and, as we shall
presently see, are limited to the dorso-lateral part of the
neural wall. They can be distinctly seen in the last two
stages (8- and 11-somite) of figure 3, but can be followed
better in figures 4 and 5.
PRIMARY SUBDIVISIONS OF BRAIN
To draw conclusions solely from dorsal views of the developing brain has its dangers. From a casual glance at the
8—somite stage in figure 3, one might say that here are three
brain vesicles. If, however, one splits the brain along the
middle line and studies the median sagittal View (fig. 4,
8-somite), he finds that from that aspect an actual division
into brain vesicles does not exist. In.stead of an anterior
brain vesicle, one sees one of the optic vesicles projecting
laterally, separated from its mate by the incompletely fused
thick dorsal margins of the neural plates. It is not a single
uniform common cavity but one already bilaterally specialized
into parts. Lying next and caudal to this is a surface where
the thalamus is to appear. In the dorsal view this in some
specimens is partially obscured by the bulging eye vesicles.
Back of this in turn comes the enlarging wall that 011 each
side is to go into the optic lobe. finally there can be recognized slight bulging areas that are forming the nuclei of
origin of the trigeminal and the acoustico-facial nerves.
Caudal to this point the neural plates are still open.
Surfaces and contours which are subtle in the 8-somite
stage become distinct in the 11-somite stage. With suitable
illumination and manipulation of shadows one can emphasize
the depressed areas where the neural wall bulges outward,
and this means the areas where proliferation is more active.
At 17-somites there can be no further question as to the contours of these foci of development and they can be readily
traced into the older stages with constant addition of new
areas or subdivisions of the earlier ones. figures 4 and 5
present a series of stages in which these are labeled and they
can be followed step by step as the brain grows in size and
METAMERISM IN CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM 467
in complexity. It is 11ot necessary to describe all these in
detail here, but it will be noted that there is an absence of
serial repetition or uniformity among them, such as is demanded by metamerism. It is true that they are arranged
along a longitudinal axis which could scarcely be otherwise
in an elongated organism, but otherwise their individuality
and appropriateness in position and size for particular purposes are their main characteristics. In this connection it
may be pointed out that the central nervous system is nicely
adapted to the structures it serves, from the very early stages.
It is adapted to the body just as much as the body is adapted
to it. Furthermore, in its early stages it is adapted to the
simple embryonic requirements which are chiefly the muscle
primordia, and as the requirements of the organism become
more complex the nervous system keeps abreast. If this is
kept in mind one can better interpret the early morphology
of the neural axis. As an illustration of this point, there is
shown in figure 6 a sketch illustrating how in the region of
the segmented mesoderm the neural axis conforms to the
consequent requirements, whereas, in the region of the facial
processes and branchial bars there is supplied an appropriate
set of cranial nerves. Further forward the primary require»
ments are the visual and olfactory mechanisms, which obligations the brain meets early and adequately. It can be understood how subsequently in rapid succession new centers and
super-centers made their appearance for control, elaboration,
and coordination of the brain itself. These with their various communications find their places efiiciently and with striking economy of space. It is apparently in such terms that
the transformation of the simpler embryonic form into the
final brain must be interpreted. This is quite opposite to the
conception that a metameric tube after the fashion of Hill
must first be subdivided into three homogeneous brain—vesicles
which then become further subdivided into the definite parts.
For the latter view the writer finds no confirmation in the
brain of the chick.
468 G. L. STREETER


Thai.
Segmentation of the vertebrate body seems to be originally limited to the middle germ—layer and Where segmental arrangement occurs in derivatives of the ectoderm it is of secondary origin and is an adaptation to the metamerism of the mesoblast.


 
Photographic records of the brain of the 4—sornite chick do not reveal the segmented condition pictured by Hill (fig. 1), nor do the next older or younger stages exhibit constrictions that can be satisfactorily fitted in with such a system of uniform segmentation. Instead of definable metameres which can be traced into specific brain parts, it is found that certain of the functional subdivisions of the brain reveal their identity Very early and before the closure of the neural folds. As development progresses, new subdivisions and modifying mechanisms detach from these or appear in the intervals between them. To what extent these more tardy subdivisions are predetermined structures and to what extent they are the product of interaction between the pioneer mechanisms is beyond the province of this paper. Morphologically, however, the various nuclear masses as they successively make their appearance and the communicating fiber-laminae can be traced step by step as they individually grow in size and complexity until the final form is attained. All of this takes place in the absence of any true metameric segmentation. Qualification should perhaps be made for the visceral cranial nerves. That portion of the neural axis from which they arise is characterized by marked transverse grooves, and if this group of nerves is to be regarded as segmental then the grooves produced by the proliferating neuroblasts giving origin to the nerves must also be held as segmental. But that is not granting much to neural metamerism.






Basal Piafe


   
fig.1 Brain segments in the 4—somito chick according to H.ill (1899). This frequently copied figure illustrates neural segmentation with great daring. When this is compared with photographs of actual specimens (fig. 2), scant justification is found for the segmentation as shown. Only the somite grooves are found in the photographs.


 


V”.


 
 


‘ Opflc
fig.2 Ventral and dorsal views showing tlm neural folds in four chick embryos of about the 4-somite stage for c-,omp:1rison with figure 1. Flnlargmnmut, X 20.
recess




Hemisph.
==Chick Brain at Closure of Neural Tube==


 
The chick is one of the most thoroughly studied of all embryonic types. It has been elaborately and systematically portrayed in atlas form and, moreover, in a whole series of atlases, beautiful and well known.‘ But even in this abundance of material it is difficult to determine whether I-Iill’s evanescent segments of the brain are present or not. They cannot be seen satisfactorily in total cleared preparations studied by transmitted light, the method heretofore invariably employed. In such specimens one has only incomplete glimpses of surfaces, rendered uncertain by refraction and overlapping of structures and inaccessible to special illumination. Consequently, a large personal equation enters in the selection of significant levels and contours. Nor do serial sections and reconstruction methods suffice. In face of these difficulties, it was concluded that the desired information as


region
‘E1-dl, M. 1’. 1845. Die Entwiekelung dos Menschen und des Hiihnchens im Eie. Leipzig. Duval, M. .1899. Atlas d’embryologie. Paris.


40 s.
Keibel, F. 12. K. Abraham. 1900. Normentafel zur Entwicklungsgeschichte des I-Iulmes. Jena.


fig.4 Dissections of the brain of the chick, enlarged 25 diameters, showing
in a series of stages the :11'e:3.s of more rapid development as judged by the
expansion of the neural wall. It will be seen that the bulging areas correspond
to primitive functional mechanisms which can be traced in figure 5 to definite


brain parts.
to the true form of the neural tube must be obtained from specimens whose tissues are rendered opaque and can be freely exposed to direct illumination by means of dissection and under circumstances that introduce the minimum amount of artifact. It was found that chick embryos having these requirements could be prepared by fixing blastoderms in Bouin’s solution which are then rinsed and dehydrated up to 80 per cent alcohol. In the latter medium they are dissected in various ways and photographed with carefully adjusted illuminations. At the stages in which we are interested the embryos are very tiny and magnification is necessary, the original negatives being made at an enlargement of 12 diameters. In making prints for study the enlargement is further increased to 25 or 50 diameters. A series of such photographs are shown in figure 2 and they will now be described.
METAMEE-ISM IN CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM 469


 
 
 
 
   


 
The four stages shown in figure 2 were selected as representing the same period in which Hill found such well-defined brain segments and they should be closely compared with his figure reproduced in figure 1. At this time the neural folds are just beginning to close and there are from 3 to 4 separated somites. A and A’ of figure 2 are dorsal and ventral views of the youngest specimen in the group. The endoderm and heart have been removed, exposing the ventral surface of the primary sheet of mesoderm (A’). Hensen’s node can be seen, and extending forward from it is the flat notochord. Lateral to the notochord on each side are the stripsiof the thickened mesoderm from which the somites are being segmented, three pairs can already be seen. The dorsal View of the same specimen (A) shows the neural folds as still rather flat, except at the front end where there is active proliferation of the constituent cells and a relative excess of tissue, particularly in theregion where the eye evagination is to take place. The proliferation of cells occurs in such a way that their margins are turned up and tend to approximate. In gross appearance it is as though the opposing plates were pressed together with thumb and finger. The point to be noticed is that there is no distinguishable segmentation, except in the region of the mesodermal somites, which appear to be the elements responsible for the phenomenon. There are slight irregularities elsewhere along the margins of the folds, but they are less marked and bear no more evidence of being segmental than those characterizing the margins of the primitive groove in this same specimen.


Co rp. sf riafum


&»j‘l\01fac’r.reqFon
The next specimen (fig. 2, B and B’) is slightly older. The primitive pit is more distinct and at that point on the ventral surface there is a prominent Hensen’s node, set apart from t.he primitive streak caudal to it. The fourth pair of mesodermal somites ca11 be recognized. The notochord can be seen lyi11g along the mid-line and taking its origin from Hensen’s node. The neural plates lie flat as in the 3—somite stage, except at the front end where they are bent upward and their margins pressed against each other. In the ventral view (B’) the neural axis is correspondingly narrower, although there is already a little‘ fullness where the optic evagination is to take place. As in the previous specimen, there is no evidence of orderly segmentation other than that concerned with the somites. Nor is there so far, if we adhere to the material evidence before us, anything in the way of a brain vesicle.
46-475. 3 days x I2‘


 
   
 


Pamphys.
The next two specimens (fig. 2, C and D) are essentially representatives of the same stage as the last specimen, showing variants in form. In one of them (0') the optic evagination is more advanced than in B’ and in the other |D’) it is less advanced. Otherwise the closing neural tube is smooth and devoid of segmentation, excepting in the region of the somites. That the mesodermal somi.tes influence the form of the neural ectoderm with which they are in contact can be seen by stripping off the mesoderm, as was done in D’. Here one can speak at least of secondary segmentation. Whether these neural segments would form in the absence of mesodermal somites must be determined before the question can be answered as to their primary or secondary nature. Elsewhere the neural plates during this period are devoid of any such phenomenon. Nor do these photographs show as yet any brain vesicles.


 


Opfic recess
The outline drawings in figure 3 represent the initial phases in the closure of the neural tube. As we shall see, separation of the tube into subdivisions begins even as early as this and the effort has been made to record the form of the tube as accurately as possible. The contours were obtained by making tracings of enlarged photographs. Thus, the 3- and 4-somite stages of figure 3 are from the same specimens shown in figure 2, A and B, with which they should be compared. The others were made from similar photographs of various other specimens. In all cases the drawings were controlled by the original embryonic material under the binocular microscope. The drawings were all made at the same enlargement so that one can follow any alterations in width or length. For their final rendering in ink I am indebted to the expert hand of Mr. J. F. Didusch.
Corp sfridrum


505. 472 days x :2
Mefafhai.


EP‘P'W?,
In comparing these stages the transformation of the caudal part of the germ—disk can be dismissed with few words. This is the zone of primary differentiation where, through the activity of Hensen’s node and the contiguous primitive streak, certain elements, such as mesoderm and notochord, are sepa~ rated out from the ectodermal caudal germ—bed and there is left in the path behind, or rather in front, the median longitudinal chordal scar, 011 each side of which are the paired 11eura.l plates which are to become the central nervous system. This constitutes the primary exodus of non-nervous elements from the ectoderm. As growth proceeds, the zone where this neural purification is taking place is found progressively further and further back, remaining characteristically, to the very last, flat and open just as was the original germ-disk.


 
 
   
 
   
     


C°YP- mum Cerebeflum
Confining our attention to the purified neural plates, it will be seen that their closure into a tube begins in the chick, near the eye region and is effected by the bending up of the plates so that their margins are pressed together and the same process spreads from this region caudalward simultaneously
Anfilobe Hyp. (1 9 fie 5 Q
Chnusrnag 7da_y5 X 8  ”>
Opfic recess \ W
Corp.5h*Ia+urn \ ’/


Opffc lobe


 


Epiphysls
fig.3 Outline dr:1wi11gs of’ the chick brain during the period of c011versi011 into 3. 11011172.! tube, showing certain precocious primitive parts \vhic,h already 9:411 be recognized {it that time. These structures determine the contours of the neural tube. It will be seen that lnetameric segmentation is present only in the region of the mesodermal somites. The specimens are all enlarged 25 diameters.


Olflacf. bufb


     
with the elongation of the neural axis. At first the margins merely press against each other. This is the condition up to the 6- and 7-somite stages. At the 8-somite stage actual fusion has taken place along the line where the first contact occurred. From this point the phenomenon of closure or fusion of the mid-dorsal seam spreads forward and caudalward and in doing so gives rise to the neural crest which will not be dealt with here. That much will be generally granted. But while the tube is closing there is active proliferation of the cells composing its wall. This proliferation, like the closure itself, occurs earlier at the oral end, spreading thence backward, and, furthermore, it is more active in certain zones resulting in a fullness or expansion of the corresponding areas, characteristic for the stage and the species. It is the significance of the consequent irregularities in contour of the neural tube which I wish to raise in question. I cannot see that these are orderly uniform metameric segments as pietured by Hill, instead they appear to be definite specialized structures, the primordia of the more primitive mechanisms, which can be followed through. into the completed brain. As an example of a definitive part of the brain, whose primordium can be recognized very early in the embryo, the chick offers a very conspicuous one in the visual apparatus. Already in the 3- and 4-somite stages there is a fullness in the region where the eye is to appear. At the 6-somite stage, as can be seen in figure 3, there is on each side a well-demarcated evagination expanding laterally. From then on these bilateral evaginations are unmistakably the eyes. It is to be noted that neither dorsally, anteriorly, nor ventrally do these meet in the middle line. In other words, in the 6- to 7-somite stage we are not dealing with a primary brain vesicle, but with two eye vesicles separated by a part of the brain Wall that is to form other important structures less far advanced. Furthermore, the eye vesicles are there before the dorsal margins actually come together and before one could postulate a primary brain vesicle. Following closely after the formation of the eye vesicles are the optic thalami and the optic lobes which are also essentially bilateral and, as we shall presently see, are limited to the dorso-lateral part of the neural wall. They can be distinctly seen in the last two stages (8- and 11-somite) of figure 3, but can be followed better in figures 4 and 5.


Brfdqe connecfinq Q
==Primary Subdivisions of Brain==


ln+erven+r'icular op“: lobes
To draw conclusions solely from dorsal views of the developing brain has its dangers. From a casual glance at the 8—somite stage in figure 3, one might say that here are three brain vesicles. If, however, one splits the brain along the middle line and studies the median sagittal View (fig. 4, 8-somite), he finds that from that aspect an actual division into brain vesicles does not exist. In.stead of an anterior brain vesicle, one sees one of the optic vesicles projecting laterally, separated from its mate by the incompletely fused thick dorsal margins of the neural plates. It is not a single uniform common cavity but one already bilaterally specialized into parts. Lying next and caudal to this is a surface where the thalamus is to appear. In the dorsal view this in some specimens is partially obscured by the bulging eye vesicles. Back of this in turn comes the enlarging wall that 011 each side is to go into the optic lobe. finally there can be recognized slight bulging areas that are forming the nuclei of origin of the trigeminal and the acoustico-facial nerves. Caudal to this point the neural plates are still open.


passage
Posi: com.


14 days x 8
Surfaces and contours which are subtle in the 8-somite stage become distinct in the 11-somite stage. With suitable illumination and manipulation of shadows one can emphasize the depressed areas where the neural wall bulges outward, and this means the areas where proliferation is more active. At 17-somites there can be no further question as to the contours of these foci of development and they can be readily traced into the older stages with constant addition of new areas or subdivisions of the earlier ones. figures 4 and 5 present a series of stages in which these are labeled and they can be followed step by step as the brain grows in size and in complexity. It is 11ot necessary to describe all these in detail here, but it will be noted that there is an absence of serial repetition or uniformity among them, such as is demanded by metamerism. It is true that they are arranged along a longitudinal axis which could scarcely be otherwise in an elongated organism, but otherwise their individuality and appropriateness in position and size for particular purposes are their main characteristics. In this connection it may be pointed out that the central nervous system is nicely adapted to the structures it serves, from the very early stages. It is adapted to the body just as much as the body is adapted to it. Furthermore, in its early stages it is adapted to the simple embryonic requirements which are chiefly the muscle primordia, and as the requirements of the organism become more complex the nervous system keeps abreast. If this is kept in mind one can better interpret the early morphology of the neural axis. As an illustration of this point, there is shown in figure 6 a sketch illustrating how in the region of the segmented mesoderm the neural axis conforms to the consequent requirements, whereas, in the region of the facial processes and branchial bars there is supplied an appropriate set of cranial nerves. Further forward the primary require» ments are the visual and olfactory mechanisms, which obligations the brain meets early and adequately. It can be understood how subsequently in rapid succession new centers and super-centers made their appearance for control, elaboration, and coordination of the brain itself. These with their various communications find their places efiiciently and with striking economy of space. It is apparently in such terms that the transformation of the simpler embryonic form into the final brain must be interpreted. This is quite opposite to the conception that a metameric tube after the fashion of Hill must first be subdivided into three homogeneous brain—vesicles which then become further subdivided into the definite parts.


Corp, mum.
For the latter view the writer finds no confirmation in the brain of the chick.  


Hypsphysis
fig.4 Dissections of the brain of the chick, enlarged 25 diameters, showing in a series of stages the :11'e:3.s of more rapid development as judged by the expansion of the neural wall. It will be seen that the bulging areas correspond to primitive functional mechanisms which can be traced in figure 5 to definite


brain parts.


Lamina +ermI'na||'s


Opficrecess Chiasma
fig.5 A continuation of figure 4. The en1a.rgement for each specimen is specified. 470 G. L. STREETER


fig.5 A continuation of figure 4. The en1a.rgement for each specimen is
The Visual apparatus which is such a dominating feature of the chick brain is a favorable structure to follow through the stages shown in figures 4 and 5. It is a good representative of the principle of early determination of the various regions of the neural tube. As early as the 11-somite
specified.
470 G. L. STREETER


The Visual apparatus which is such a dominating feature
of the chick brain is a favorable structure to follow through
the stages shown in figures 4 and 5. It is a good representative of the principle of early determination of the various regions of the neural tube. As early as the 11-somite


Adapted to


/ branchial system
fig. 6 Outline drawing of centrzil nervous system of a 25~s0mite human embryo (Carnegie Collection, No. 6097). The somites and the condensed tissues of the branchial region are the only body parts that seem to be calling for help from the nervous system and the form of the neural tube is nicely fitted to their service. The character of segmentation shown by them is correspondingly reflected in the neural tube. Otherwise, the environment at this time offers the brain no inducements to segmentation. The endothelial apparatus is at first quite indi— pendent of the nervous system.




fig. 6 Outline drawing of centrzil nervous system of a 25~s0mite human
stage the eye vesicle begins to acquire a stalk—like constriction and at 17 somites one may speak of an optic foramen. This actually diminishes in size up to the 21—somite stage and then, due to growth of the structures in the adjoining wall, it is converted into the optic recess and becomes finally a mere slit. Of the other parts of the Visual apparatus the thalamus and optic lobe appear early. The older anatomists spoke of the ‘optic thalamus.’ In the embryo the relation between the thalamus and the optic evagination is very close, being contiguous at the 8—somite stage. This continuity is later expressed in the optic tract. The optic lobe as a terminal visual center undergoes an extraordinary growth. As is true for the cerebral hemisphere in man, it first establishes a great surface expansion. This mechanically favors the subsequent stratification and correlating fiber sheets which characterize the later thickening of the wall. In the 3- and 4-day chick the right and left optic lobes open widely into each other and give the appearance of being one optic lobe, which we all delight to call the mid-brain vesicle. In reality the optic lobes start as bilateral expansion of the neural wall and maintain their bilaterality throughout. This is conspicuously true in the chick where in adaptation to the available space the two lobes become separated like saddle-bags on the sides of the brain-stem, connection being maintained by a broad lamina of commissural fibers.
embryo (Carnegie Collection, No. 6097). The somites and the condensed tissues
of the branchial region are the only body parts that seem to be calling for help
from the nervous system and the form of the neural tube is nicely fitted to their
service. The character of segmentation shown by them is correspondingly reflected
in the neural tube. Otherwise, the environment at this time offers the brain no
inducements to segmentation. The endothelial apparatus is at first quite indi—
pendent of the nervous system.


stage the eye vesicle begins to acquire a stalk—like constriction and at 17 somites one may speak of an optic foramen.
This actually diminishes in size up to the 21—somite stage and
then, due to growth of the structures in the adjoining wall,
it is converted into the optic recess and becomes finally a
mere slit. Of the other parts of the Visual apparatus the
thalamus and optic lobe appear early. The older anatomists
METAMERISM IN CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEAI


spoke of the ‘optic thalamus.’ In the embryo the relation
In studying the optic lobes one can see that they are distinct and can be demarcated ventrally from the basal plate. This basal plate is destined to become an important part of the brain. It becomes the main connecting trunk of the brainstem. It is the cerebral ‘main street’ or an elongated ‘market’ along Whose margins are the great nerve centers from which it receives and to which it distributes fibers. While its primary purpose appears to be concerned with traffic, various effecting, regulating, and correlating centers are interspersed along its course. From the point of View of this paper it is to be noted that it has an identity and that it is a longitudinal structure which is not interrupted by transverse partitions such as would be essential in a system of three brain vesicles. To draw a line across it as we have been accustomed to do in the past, saying what is in front of the line is one thing and what is behind it another, is an act of rank pedagogic violence. Anteriorly, the basal plate disappears below the thalamus and corpus striatum blending with the great fiber systems which come from them and course on their lateral surfaces. With its extension caudalward the basal plate undergoes enormous growth throughout the peduncular and pontine regions where it will be recognized as the formatio reticularis which in turn is continuous with the large ground bundles of the spinal cord. The term ‘peduncles of the brain’ has been granted by custom to specific bundles of fibers which have not reached the basal plate in the earlier stages. Otherwise one might wish to use the term ‘pedunculus cerebri’ instead of basal plate for this structure from the beginning. This, however, is not the place to discuss the terminology of this part of the adult brain for which there is so great an opportunity-»a region where we yet speak of a ‘tcctum’ of the ‘tegmentum.
between the thalamus and the optic evagination is very close,
being contiguous at the 8—somite stage. This continuity is
later expressed in the optic tract. The optic lobe as a terminal
visual center undergoes an extraordinary growth. As is true
for the cerebral hemisphere in man, it first establishes a great
surface expansion. This mechanically favors the subsequent
stratification and correlating fiber sheets which characterize
the later thickening of the wall. In the 3- and 4-day chick
the right and left optic lobes open widely into each other and
give the appearance of being one optic lobe, which we all
delight to call the mid-brain vesicle. In reality the optic
lobes start as bilateral expansion of the neural wall and
maintain their bilaterality throughout. This is conspicuously
true in the chick where in adaptation to the available space
the two lobes become separated like saddle-bags on the sides
of the brain-stem, connection being maintained by a broad
lamina of commissural fibers.


In studying the optic lobes one can see that they are distinct
and can be demarcated ventrally from the basal plate. This
basal plate is destined to become an important part of the
brain. It becomes the main connecting trunk of the brainstem. It is the cerebral ‘main street’ or an elongated
‘market’ along Whose margins are the great nerve centers
from which it receives and to which it distributes fibers.
While its primary purpose appears to be concerned with
traffic, various effecting, regulating, and correlating centers
are interspersed along its course. From the point of View
of this paper it is to be noted that it has an identity and
that it is a longitudinal structure which is not interrupted
by transverse partitions such as would be essential in a
system of three brain vesicles. To draw a line across it as
we have been accustomed to do in the past, saying what is in
front of the line is one thing and what is behind it another,
is an act of rank pedagogic violence. Anteriorly, the basal
plate disappears below the thalamus and corpus striatum
blending with the great fiber systems which come from them
472 G. L. STREETER


and course on their lateral surfaces. With its extension
Between the optic lobe and the thalamus a new subdivision or focal area of active proliferation appears at the 21-somite stage, and this has been designated as the metathalamus abiding the time when a more specific designation can be arrived at. It appears to correspond to a group of pretectal nuclei.
caudalward the basal plate undergoes enormous growth
throughout the peduncular and pontine regions where it will
be recognized as the formatio reticularis which in turn is
continuous with the large ground bundles of the spinal cord.
The term ‘peduncles of the brain’ has been granted by custom
to specific bundles of fibers which have not reached the basal
plate in the earlier stages. Otherwise one might wish to use
the term ‘pedunculus cerebri’ instead of basal plate for this
structure from the beginning. This, however, is not the place
to discuss the terminology of this part of the adult brain for
which there is so great an opportunity-»a region where we
yet speak of a ‘tcctum’ of the ‘tegmentum.


Between the optic lobe and the thalamus a new subdivision
or focal area of active proliferation appears at the 21-somite
stage, and this has been designated as the metathalamus abiding the time when a more specific designation can be arrived
at. It appears to correspond to a group of pretectal nuclei.


A conspicuous feature of the early neural tube is a segmental-like effect which is associated with the innervation
A conspicuous feature of the early neural tube is a segmental-like effect which is associated with the innervation of the facial processes and the branchial bars. Already. at the 8- and 11—somite stages one can see expansions of the neural wall at foci of active proliferation producing transverse grooves and intervening ridges. With some variation between Vertebrate classes and orders, there is a general similarity in the existence of these grooves among all verte— bratcs. The grooves in the chick are not quite the same as those in pig and man, but as in these two orders they show a constant relationship to particular visceral cranial nerves. The points at which the respective nerves emerge from the neural wall and their relation to the grooves were plotted as carefully as possible and are shown for the successive stages in figures 4 and 5. It is to be noted that the different grooves possess certain individualities, in shape, width, length, and point of nerve exit. Furthermore, there are more grooves than nerves. The first two must be allotted to the trigeminal nerve and there is no nerve for the groove between the facial and glossopharyngeal nerves unless it be the acoustic nerve Which is not a visceral nerve. None of the grooves extend across the median line, even at their maximum develop ment at the end of the second day, being interrupted by the median raphe. The basal plate gradually spreads out ventral to them and as it increases in mass, being unsegmental itself, the grooves gradually disappear. These grooves are much more marked and appear to have no continuity with the type of segmentation that characterizes the neural tubes opposite the somites, which is quite contrary to the conclusion arrived at by Hill. Peripherally, the visceral cranial nerves correspond precisely to the condensed cell masses they innervate, as can be seen in figure 6, and it seems probable that their growth is a neural response to their particular environment. They would therefore be expected to exhibit the same type of segmentation prevailing there, which is a very different sort from that of the somites.
of the facial processes and the branchial bars. Already. at
the 8- and 11—somite stages one can see expansions of the
neural wall at foci of active proliferation producing transverse grooves and intervening ridges. With some variation
between Vertebrate classes and orders, there is a general
similarity in the existence of these grooves among all verte—
bratcs. The grooves in the chick are not quite the same as
those in pig and man, but as in these two orders they show
a constant relationship to particular visceral cranial nerves.
The points at which the respective nerves emerge from the
neural wall and their relation to the grooves were plotted
as carefully as possible and are shown for the successive
stages in figures 4 and 5. It is to be noted that the different
grooves possess certain individualities, in shape, width,
length, and point of nerve exit. Furthermore, there are more
grooves than nerves. The first two must be allotted to the
trigeminal nerve and there is no nerve for the groove between
METAMERISM IN CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM 473


the facial and glossopharyngeal nerves unless it be the acoustic nerve Which is not a visceral nerve. None of the grooves
==Summary==
extend across the median line, even at their maximum develop
ment at the end of the second day, being interrupted by the
median raphe. The basal plate gradually spreads out ventral to them and as it increases in mass, being unsegmental
itself, the grooves gradually disappear. These grooves are
much more marked and appear to have no continuity with the
type of segmentation that characterizes the neural tubes opposite the somites, which is quite contrary to the conclusion
arrived at by Hill. Peripherally, the visceral cranial nerves
correspond precisely to the condensed cell masses they innervate, as can be seen in figure 6, and it seems probable that
their growth is a neural response to their particular environment. They would therefore be expected to exhibit the same
type of segmentation prevailing there, which is a very different sort from that of the somites.


SUMMARY
From morphological evidence one must conclude that the nearest approach to metamerism in the embryonic central nervous system is that occurring in the spinal region, and even this appears to be an adaptation to the Somites that lie alongside. The segmentation is evidently primary in the Somites, which during the early stages make an impress on the cord. ‘


From morphological evidence one must conclude that the
A different type of segmentation is found in the branchial region——a type that is not continuous with and is different in histological character from the somite—type of the cord. The condensed cellular masses that are to form the tissues and musculature of the maxillary, mandibular, and throat regions receive appropriate and early innervation from a series of nerves, the visceral cranial nerves, which are clearly associated with a series of grooves in the neural Wall. These bilaterally placed transverse grooves are the expression of active cell proliferation and represent foci of origin with the associated nuclei, of these visceral cranial nerves. Although resembling each other in general, these grooves possess individualities which distinguish them from one another, and in the different vertebrate orders there are certain variations in their finer morphological details. The trigeminal and facial are the first to establish themselves.
nearest approach to metamerism in the embryonic central
nervous system is that occurring in the spinal region, and
even this appears to be an adaptation to the Somites that lie
alongside. The segmentation is evidently primary in the
Somites, which during the early stages make an impress on
the cord.


A different type of segmentation is found in the branchial
region——a type that is not continuous with and is different in
histological character from the somite—type of the cord. The
condensed cellular masses that are to form the tissues and
musculature of the maxillary, mandibular, and throat regions
receive appropriate and early innervation from a series of
nerves, the visceral cranial nerves, which are clearly associated with a series of grooves in the neural Wall. These
bilaterally placed transverse grooves are the expression of
active cell proliferation and represent foci of origin with the
associated nuclei, of these visceral cranial nerves. Although
474 G. L. STBEETER


resembling each other in general, these grooves possess individualities which distinguish them from one another, and in
A third category of ‘segmentation’ is brought about by expansions of the neural wall with intervening constrictions associated with the development of definite brain parts. These subdivisions give evidence of early determination of the more fundamental structures which can be followed through to their final form in the mature brain. From the outset they are characterized in each case by individuality of form and are not serial repetitions of a uniform pattern. Reexamination of similar material does not substantiate the well-known figure of Hill showing a neural tube, metameric from stem to stern. Instead of a rigid metameric system, the neural tube shows itself responsive at all levels to its environment. VVhere the environment is truly segmented the tube takes on that character in some degree. At levels where the environment is branchiomerie there we find the neural tube responding with suitable cranial nerves. At levels still further forward, aside for certain special-sense organs, there appear to be no environmental demands, at least in the early stages, and there the neural tube devotes itself to its own requirements in the way of centers of correlation and control, the subdivision of which bears no resemblance to true segmentation.
the different vertebrate orders there are certain variations
in their finer morphological details. The trigeminal and
facial are the first to establish themselves.


A third category of ‘segmentation’ is brought about by
expansions of the neural wall with intervening constrictions
associated with the development of definite brain parts. These
subdivisions give evidence of early determination of the more
fundamental structures which can be followed through to
their final form in the mature brain. From the outset they
are characterized in each case by individuality of form and
are not serial repetitions of a uniform pattern. Reexamination of similar material does not substantiate the well-known
figure of Hill showing a neural tube, metameric from stem
to stern. Instead of a rigid metameric system, the neural
tube shows itself responsive at all levels to its environment.
VVhere the environment is truly segmented the tube takes on
that character in some degree. At levels where the environment is branchiomerie there we find the neural tube responding with suitable cranial nerves. At levels still further forward, aside for certain special-sense organs, there appear to
be no environmental demands, at least in the early stages,
and there the neural tube devotes itself to its own requirements in the way of centers of correlation and control, the
subdivision of which bears no resemblance to true segmentation.


The subdivision of the embryonic brain into three primary
The subdivision of the embryonic brain into three primary brain vesicles is an arbitrary expedient rather than a natural phenomenon. The scheme is not to be blamed on His. 'When he and his associates on the Committee for revision of Anatomical Nomenclature utilized these divisions of the brain and their subdivisions, it was done only as an aid in regional localization. No such far-reaching interpretation was inferred as that of the metameric enthusiasts. There is a certain convenience in the terms fore-brain, mid-brain, and hindbrain, and if it is remembered that there never are three primary vesicles serially equivalent and precisely demarcated, corresponding to the three brain divisions, then no harm is done. If, however, the student is taught that the neural tube becomes dilated anteriorly into three equivalent homogeneous vesicles which are to become the brain, and that it is subsequent to the acquirement of their vesicular state that these become differentiated and subdivided into actual brain structures, the said student will have been taught something that he can never verify, and it is likely that his conception of organogenesis will lean more toward the principles of the tailor—shop than toward those typical of living embryonic tissue.
brain vesicles is an arbitrary expedient rather than a natural
phenomenon. The scheme is not to be blamed on His. 'When
he and his associates on the Committee for revision of Anatomical Nomenclature utilized these divisions of the brain and
their subdivisions, it was done only as an aid in regional
localization. No such far-reaching interpretation was inferred as that of the metameric enthusiasts. There is a certain convenience in the terms fore-brain, mid-brain, and hindbrain, and if it is remembered that there never are three
METAMERISM IN CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM 475


primary vesicles serially equivalent and precisely demarcated,
corresponding to the three brain divisions, then no harm is
done. If, however, the student is taught that the neural tube
becomes dilated anteriorly into three equivalent homogeneous
vesicles which are to become the brain, and that it is subsequent to the acquirement of their vesicular state that these
become differentiated and subdivided into actual brain structures, the said student will have been taught something that
he can never verify, and it is likely that his conception of
organogenesis will lean more toward the principles of the
tailor—shop than toward those typical of living embryonic
tissue.


If intellectual stimulation is the object in view, how much
If intellectual stimulation is the object in view, how much more thrilling than the three—brain—vesicle lore is the knowledge which is now available concerning the potentialities of embryonic tissues and concerning the influence of the tissues upon each other. These interactions in their general principles are demonstrable facts and may be safely intrusted to the embryological armamentarium of the student. It is inconceivable that any rigid geometrical schema could be as marvelous as the reality of the blending adaptation maintained between the embryonic nervous system and the body structures which it is to serve, as they exist at the moment, whereby the two are continuously and perfectly coordinated. Thought of in these terms, the form of the neural tube and its modifications from stage to stage take on important significance.
more thrilling than the three—brain—vesicle lore is the knowledge which is now available concerning the potentialities of
embryonic tissues and concerning the influence of the tissues
upon each other. These interactions in their general principles are demonstrable facts and may be safely intrusted
to the embryological armamentarium of the student. It is
inconceivable that any rigid geometrical schema could be as
marvelous as the reality of the blending adaptation maintained between the embryonic nervous system and the body
structures which it is to serve, as they exist at the moment,
whereby the two are continuously and perfectly coordinated.
Thought of in these terms, the form of the neural tube and its
modifications from stage to stage take on important significance.

Revision as of 09:51, 17 March 2017

Embryology - 19 Apr 2024    Facebook link Pinterest link Twitter link  Expand to Translate  
Google Translate - select your language from the list shown below (this will open a new external page)

العربية | català | 中文 | 中國傳統的 | français | Deutsche | עִברִית | हिंदी | bahasa Indonesia | italiano | 日本語 | 한국어 | မြန်မာ | Pilipino | Polskie | português | ਪੰਜਾਬੀ ਦੇ | Română | русский | Español | Swahili | Svensk | ไทย | Türkçe | اردو | ייִדיש | Tiếng Việt    These external translations are automated and may not be accurate. (More? About Translations)

Streeter GL. The status of metamerism in the central nervous system of chick embryos. (1933) J. Comp. Neural. 455-475.

Online Editor Note 
Mark Hill.jpg
This historic 1933 paper by George L. Streeter is an early description of the development of the chicken nervous system.


  Streeter Links: George Streeter | 1905 Cranial and Spinal Nerves | 1906 Membranous Labyrinth | 1908 Peripheral Nervous System 10mm Human | 1908 Cranial Nerves 10mm Human | 1912 Nervous System | 1917 Scala Tympani Scala Vestibuli and Perioticular Cistern | 1917 Ear Cartilaginous Capsule | 1918 Otic Capsule | 1919 Filum Terminale | 1920 Presomite Embryo | 1920 Human Embryo Growth | 1921 Brain Vascular | 1938 Early Primate Stages | 1941 Macaque embryo | 1945 Stage 13-14 | 1948 Stages 15-18 | 1949 Cartilage and Bone | 1951 Stages 19-23 | Contributions to Embryology | Historic Embryology Papers | Carnegie Stages | Category:George Streeter George Linius Streeter (1873-1948)


Modern Notes:
chicken

Neural Links: ectoderm | neural | neural crest | ventricular | sensory | Stage 22 | gliogenesis | neural fetal | Medicine Lecture - Neural | Lecture - Ectoderm | Lecture - Neural Crest | Lab - Early Neural | neural abnormalities | folic acid | iodine deficiency | Fetal Alcohol Syndrome | neural postnatal | neural examination | Histology | Historic Neural | Category:Neural
Historic Disclaimer - information about historic embryology pages 
Mark Hill.jpg
Pages where the terms "Historic" (textbooks, papers, people, recommendations) appear on this site, and sections within pages where this disclaimer appears, indicate that the content and scientific understanding are specific to the time of publication. This means that while some scientific descriptions are still accurate, the terminology and interpretation of the developmental mechanisms reflect the understanding at the time of original publication and those of the preceding periods, these terms, interpretations and recommendations may not reflect our current scientific understanding.     (More? Embryology History | Historic Embryology Papers)

The Status of Metamerism in the Central Nervous System of Chick Embryos

George Linius Streeter (1873-1948)

By George L. Streeter

Department of Embryology, Carnegie Institution of Washington, Baltimore, Maryland

Six Figures (1933)

Introduction

In the laboratory in which these lines are being written there has been installed recently a memorial exhibit in commemoration of Wilhelm His.‘ It was His who first called attention to the importance of the establishment of an institute for human embryology and who formulated the general plan upon which such an institute should be conducted. It was left for Mall, a devoted apostle, to make the His dream a reality and to guide its organization as it came into existence under the form of the Department of Embryology of the Carnegie Institution of Washington. It is left to us and succeeding generations to pause before this exhibit as a shrine and contemplate the contributions of a great scientist and to rejoice in the fine friendship that existed between him a.nd one of our own great men.


  • The His historical exhibit was made possible through Prof. W. His, his son, who had in his possession the majority of the items and who has deposited them in the Carnegie Laboratory for the purposes of the memorial. There are included a large number of personal copies of his monographs and separates containing marginal notes and sketches; original manuscript of his important paper “Héiute und Hiihlen des Korpers”; lecture notebook, recorded by W. Braunc, in which are his famous lecture—drawings; microscope and lenses used by Professor His from 1856 to 1904; ombryograph invented by him and with which much of his work was done, the predecessor of the camera lucida; a box of photographic transparencies made by him and on which the illustrations in the “Atlas of human embryos” were based; comparative embryological collection of more than 1000 slides prepared by him, covering a wide range of animal forms and the original material on which many of his researches were made; various portraitphotographs and a copy of the Hans Olde etching.


One of the fields of investigation to which His made important eontributions extend over the same ground with which this paper is concerned, namely, the natural subdivisions of the neural tube. VVhile 11ot regarded as one of his major accomplishments, the subdivisions introduced by him in the “N omina anatomica” have come i11to universal use. From careful studies of the development of the human brain and the brain of embryos of other vertebrate classes, he arrived at subdivisions that are applicable to embryonic stages of the various animals as Well as to the adult brain. He provided the anatomist with a morphological analysis of the brain based on the earliest stages and determined the relations of the separate parts to one another and traced their subsequent increasing complexity of form. In this way he greatly simplified neural topography and advanced our understanding of a most intricate organ.


During the time that His and the other members of his committee, Krause and Waldeyer, were conservatively arriving at brain analysis and terminology based on demonstrable morphological realities, a different direction was taken by the comparative embryologists. Apparently intoxicated by the simplification of the mechanics of development that seemed to be offered by the principle of metamerism, they did not stop their metamerism with the middle germ layer, but were applying it also to the other tissues including the medullary tube. Several investigators in this country joined in this movement. Among them was Dr. Charles Hill, working in the Zoological Laboratory of Northwestern University and under the influence of Professor Locy? I mention Hill because of the wide use that has been made of his observations. His drawings of the early stages of the brain and his presentation of neural metamerism is in general harmony with the present prevailing views among teachers and writers in this field.

2 Hill, C. 1899. Primary segments of the vertebrate head. Anat. Anz., Bd. 16,

S. 353-369. Also, 1900, Developmental hitory of primary segments of the vertebrate head. Zool. Jahrb., Bd. 13, S. 393-446.


In brief, the Hill-Locy interpretation of metamerism in the embryonic brain holds that the encephalic portion of the neural axis is divided into some eleven ‘similar joints or segments.’ In the fore- and mid-brain these are transient, being soon obliterated and succeeded by secondary modifications or expansions. In the hind-brain, however, the primitive segments persist longer and thus for a time are contemporaneous and might be mistaken as being in series with the secondary expansions of the fore- and mid-brain. It is emphasized that they are not really coordinate with the latter, but are identical with the evanescent segments which have faded out in the more oral levels.

In his papers of 1899 and 1900, Hill published a series of drawings of salmon and chick embryos in which neural segmentation as interpreted by him is shown with diagrammatic clarity. Those of the chick are the ones that have been so frequently reproduced in books on embryology, and as typical examples I am herewith showing two of his drawings (fig. 1). They portray the 4-somite stage, at which time he finds the Whole gamut of segments in their maximum distinctness. Since the principle of neural metamerism is so largely based on this and similar evidence, it becomes important to make certain of the morphological accuracy of the underlying observations. If the neural tube is actually segmented as Hill pictures it, one must accept some such interpretation as he and the other segmentalists have elaborated.

It is with this thought that the writer has gone back to the chick embryo and, through utilization of the available improvements in morphological technique, has sought to verify the presence or absence of transitory metameric segmentation of the brain. The results of this study are reported in the following paper. In summarizing these the writer finds himself in full agreement with Froriep,"* who, 40 years ago, expressed the opinion that metameres of the medullary tube, in the sense of primitive members of a segmentally laid-down organ system, are not justified by the available evidence.

‘Froriep, A. 1892. Verhandl. Anat. Gesel1., Wien, Ergi1'.nzungsh., Anat. Anz.

Segmentation of the vertebrate body seems to be originally limited to the middle germ—layer and Where segmental arrangement occurs in derivatives of the ectoderm it is of secondary origin and is an adaptation to the metamerism of the mesoblast.

Photographic records of the brain of the 4—sornite chick do not reveal the segmented condition pictured by Hill (fig. 1), nor do the next older or younger stages exhibit constrictions that can be satisfactorily fitted in with such a system of uniform segmentation. Instead of definable metameres which can be traced into specific brain parts, it is found that certain of the functional subdivisions of the brain reveal their identity Very early and before the closure of the neural folds. As development progresses, new subdivisions and modifying mechanisms detach from these or appear in the intervals between them. To what extent these more tardy subdivisions are predetermined structures and to what extent they are the product of interaction between the pioneer mechanisms is beyond the province of this paper. Morphologically, however, the various nuclear masses as they successively make their appearance and the communicating fiber-laminae can be traced step by step as they individually grow in size and complexity until the final form is attained. All of this takes place in the absence of any true metameric segmentation. Qualification should perhaps be made for the visceral cranial nerves. That portion of the neural axis from which they arise is characterized by marked transverse grooves, and if this group of nerves is to be regarded as segmental then the grooves produced by the proliferating neuroblasts giving origin to the nerves must also be held as segmental. But that is not granting much to neural metamerism.



fig.1 Brain segments in the 4—somito chick according to H.ill (1899). This frequently copied figure illustrates neural segmentation with great daring. When this is compared with photographs of actual specimens (fig. 2), scant justification is found for the segmentation as shown. Only the somite grooves are found in the photographs.



fig.2 Ventral and dorsal views showing tlm neural folds in four chick embryos of about the 4-somite stage for c-,omp:1rison with figure 1. Flnlargmnmut, X 20.


Chick Brain at Closure of Neural Tube

The chick is one of the most thoroughly studied of all embryonic types. It has been elaborately and systematically portrayed in atlas form and, moreover, in a whole series of atlases, beautiful and well known.‘ But even in this abundance of material it is difficult to determine whether I-Iill’s evanescent segments of the brain are present or not. They cannot be seen satisfactorily in total cleared preparations studied by transmitted light, the method heretofore invariably employed. In such specimens one has only incomplete glimpses of surfaces, rendered uncertain by refraction and overlapping of structures and inaccessible to special illumination. Consequently, a large personal equation enters in the selection of significant levels and contours. Nor do serial sections and reconstruction methods suffice. In face of these difficulties, it was concluded that the desired information as

‘E1-dl, M. 1’. 1845. Die Entwiekelung dos Menschen und des Hiihnchens im Eie. Leipzig. Duval, M. .1899. Atlas d’embryologie. Paris.

Keibel, F. 12. K. Abraham. 1900. Normentafel zur Entwicklungsgeschichte des I-Iulmes. Jena.


to the true form of the neural tube must be obtained from specimens whose tissues are rendered opaque and can be freely exposed to direct illumination by means of dissection and under circumstances that introduce the minimum amount of artifact. It was found that chick embryos having these requirements could be prepared by fixing blastoderms in Bouin’s solution which are then rinsed and dehydrated up to 80 per cent alcohol. In the latter medium they are dissected in various ways and photographed with carefully adjusted illuminations. At the stages in which we are interested the embryos are very tiny and magnification is necessary, the original negatives being made at an enlargement of 12 diameters. In making prints for study the enlargement is further increased to 25 or 50 diameters. A series of such photographs are shown in figure 2 and they will now be described.


The four stages shown in figure 2 were selected as representing the same period in which Hill found such well-defined brain segments and they should be closely compared with his figure reproduced in figure 1. At this time the neural folds are just beginning to close and there are from 3 to 4 separated somites. A and A’ of figure 2 are dorsal and ventral views of the youngest specimen in the group. The endoderm and heart have been removed, exposing the ventral surface of the primary sheet of mesoderm (A’). Hensen’s node can be seen, and extending forward from it is the flat notochord. Lateral to the notochord on each side are the stripsiof the thickened mesoderm from which the somites are being segmented, three pairs can already be seen. The dorsal View of the same specimen (A) shows the neural folds as still rather flat, except at the front end where there is active proliferation of the constituent cells and a relative excess of tissue, particularly in theregion where the eye evagination is to take place. The proliferation of cells occurs in such a way that their margins are turned up and tend to approximate. In gross appearance it is as though the opposing plates were pressed together with thumb and finger. The point to be noticed is that there is no distinguishable segmentation, except in the region of the mesodermal somites, which appear to be the elements responsible for the phenomenon. There are slight irregularities elsewhere along the margins of the folds, but they are less marked and bear no more evidence of being segmental than those characterizing the margins of the primitive groove in this same specimen.


The next specimen (fig. 2, B and B’) is slightly older. The primitive pit is more distinct and at that point on the ventral surface there is a prominent Hensen’s node, set apart from t.he primitive streak caudal to it. The fourth pair of mesodermal somites ca11 be recognized. The notochord can be seen lyi11g along the mid-line and taking its origin from Hensen’s node. The neural plates lie flat as in the 3—somite stage, except at the front end where they are bent upward and their margins pressed against each other. In the ventral view (B’) the neural axis is correspondingly narrower, although there is already a little‘ fullness where the optic evagination is to take place. As in the previous specimen, there is no evidence of orderly segmentation other than that concerned with the somites. Nor is there so far, if we adhere to the material evidence before us, anything in the way of a brain vesicle.


The next two specimens (fig. 2, C and D) are essentially representatives of the same stage as the last specimen, showing variants in form. In one of them (0') the optic evagination is more advanced than in B’ and in the other |D’) it is less advanced. Otherwise the closing neural tube is smooth and devoid of segmentation, excepting in the region of the somites. That the mesodermal somi.tes influence the form of the neural ectoderm with which they are in contact can be seen by stripping off the mesoderm, as was done in D’. Here one can speak at least of secondary segmentation. Whether these neural segments would form in the absence of mesodermal somites must be determined before the question can be answered as to their primary or secondary nature. Elsewhere the neural plates during this period are devoid of any such phenomenon. Nor do these photographs show as yet any brain vesicles.


The outline drawings in figure 3 represent the initial phases in the closure of the neural tube. As we shall see, separation of the tube into subdivisions begins even as early as this and the effort has been made to record the form of the tube as accurately as possible. The contours were obtained by making tracings of enlarged photographs. Thus, the 3- and 4-somite stages of figure 3 are from the same specimens shown in figure 2, A and B, with which they should be compared. The others were made from similar photographs of various other specimens. In all cases the drawings were controlled by the original embryonic material under the binocular microscope. The drawings were all made at the same enlargement so that one can follow any alterations in width or length. For their final rendering in ink I am indebted to the expert hand of Mr. J. F. Didusch.


In comparing these stages the transformation of the caudal part of the germ—disk can be dismissed with few words. This is the zone of primary differentiation where, through the activity of Hensen’s node and the contiguous primitive streak, certain elements, such as mesoderm and notochord, are sepa~ rated out from the ectodermal caudal germ—bed and there is left in the path behind, or rather in front, the median longitudinal chordal scar, 011 each side of which are the paired 11eura.l plates which are to become the central nervous system. This constitutes the primary exodus of non-nervous elements from the ectoderm. As growth proceeds, the zone where this neural purification is taking place is found progressively further and further back, remaining characteristically, to the very last, flat and open just as was the original germ-disk.


Confining our attention to the purified neural plates, it will be seen that their closure into a tube begins in the chick, near the eye region and is effected by the bending up of the plates so that their margins are pressed together and the same process spreads from this region caudalward simultaneously


fig.3 Outline dr:1wi11gs of’ the chick brain during the period of c011versi011 into 3. 11011172.! tube, showing certain precocious primitive parts \vhic,h already 9:411 be recognized {it that time. These structures determine the contours of the neural tube. It will be seen that lnetameric segmentation is present only in the region of the mesodermal somites. The specimens are all enlarged 25 diameters.


with the elongation of the neural axis. At first the margins merely press against each other. This is the condition up to the 6- and 7-somite stages. At the 8-somite stage actual fusion has taken place along the line where the first contact occurred. From this point the phenomenon of closure or fusion of the mid-dorsal seam spreads forward and caudalward and in doing so gives rise to the neural crest which will not be dealt with here. That much will be generally granted. But while the tube is closing there is active proliferation of the cells composing its wall. This proliferation, like the closure itself, occurs earlier at the oral end, spreading thence backward, and, furthermore, it is more active in certain zones resulting in a fullness or expansion of the corresponding areas, characteristic for the stage and the species. It is the significance of the consequent irregularities in contour of the neural tube which I wish to raise in question. I cannot see that these are orderly uniform metameric segments as pietured by Hill, instead they appear to be definite specialized structures, the primordia of the more primitive mechanisms, which can be followed through. into the completed brain. As an example of a definitive part of the brain, whose primordium can be recognized very early in the embryo, the chick offers a very conspicuous one in the visual apparatus. Already in the 3- and 4-somite stages there is a fullness in the region where the eye is to appear. At the 6-somite stage, as can be seen in figure 3, there is on each side a well-demarcated evagination expanding laterally. From then on these bilateral evaginations are unmistakably the eyes. It is to be noted that neither dorsally, anteriorly, nor ventrally do these meet in the middle line. In other words, in the 6- to 7-somite stage we are not dealing with a primary brain vesicle, but with two eye vesicles separated by a part of the brain Wall that is to form other important structures less far advanced. Furthermore, the eye vesicles are there before the dorsal margins actually come together and before one could postulate a primary brain vesicle. Following closely after the formation of the eye vesicles are the optic thalami and the optic lobes which are also essentially bilateral and, as we shall presently see, are limited to the dorso-lateral part of the neural wall. They can be distinctly seen in the last two stages (8- and 11-somite) of figure 3, but can be followed better in figures 4 and 5.

Primary Subdivisions of Brain

To draw conclusions solely from dorsal views of the developing brain has its dangers. From a casual glance at the 8—somite stage in figure 3, one might say that here are three brain vesicles. If, however, one splits the brain along the middle line and studies the median sagittal View (fig. 4, 8-somite), he finds that from that aspect an actual division into brain vesicles does not exist. In.stead of an anterior brain vesicle, one sees one of the optic vesicles projecting laterally, separated from its mate by the incompletely fused thick dorsal margins of the neural plates. It is not a single uniform common cavity but one already bilaterally specialized into parts. Lying next and caudal to this is a surface where the thalamus is to appear. In the dorsal view this in some specimens is partially obscured by the bulging eye vesicles. Back of this in turn comes the enlarging wall that 011 each side is to go into the optic lobe. finally there can be recognized slight bulging areas that are forming the nuclei of origin of the trigeminal and the acoustico-facial nerves. Caudal to this point the neural plates are still open.


Surfaces and contours which are subtle in the 8-somite stage become distinct in the 11-somite stage. With suitable illumination and manipulation of shadows one can emphasize the depressed areas where the neural wall bulges outward, and this means the areas where proliferation is more active. At 17-somites there can be no further question as to the contours of these foci of development and they can be readily traced into the older stages with constant addition of new areas or subdivisions of the earlier ones. figures 4 and 5 present a series of stages in which these are labeled and they can be followed step by step as the brain grows in size and in complexity. It is 11ot necessary to describe all these in detail here, but it will be noted that there is an absence of serial repetition or uniformity among them, such as is demanded by metamerism. It is true that they are arranged along a longitudinal axis which could scarcely be otherwise in an elongated organism, but otherwise their individuality and appropriateness in position and size for particular purposes are their main characteristics. In this connection it may be pointed out that the central nervous system is nicely adapted to the structures it serves, from the very early stages. It is adapted to the body just as much as the body is adapted to it. Furthermore, in its early stages it is adapted to the simple embryonic requirements which are chiefly the muscle primordia, and as the requirements of the organism become more complex the nervous system keeps abreast. If this is kept in mind one can better interpret the early morphology of the neural axis. As an illustration of this point, there is shown in figure 6 a sketch illustrating how in the region of the segmented mesoderm the neural axis conforms to the consequent requirements, whereas, in the region of the facial processes and branchial bars there is supplied an appropriate set of cranial nerves. Further forward the primary require» ments are the visual and olfactory mechanisms, which obligations the brain meets early and adequately. It can be understood how subsequently in rapid succession new centers and super-centers made their appearance for control, elaboration, and coordination of the brain itself. These with their various communications find their places efiiciently and with striking economy of space. It is apparently in such terms that the transformation of the simpler embryonic form into the final brain must be interpreted. This is quite opposite to the conception that a metameric tube after the fashion of Hill must first be subdivided into three homogeneous brain—vesicles which then become further subdivided into the definite parts.

For the latter view the writer finds no confirmation in the brain of the chick.

fig.4 Dissections of the brain of the chick, enlarged 25 diameters, showing in a series of stages the :11'e:3.s of more rapid development as judged by the expansion of the neural wall. It will be seen that the bulging areas correspond to primitive functional mechanisms which can be traced in figure 5 to definite

brain parts.


fig.5 A continuation of figure 4. The en1a.rgement for each specimen is specified. 470 G. L. STREETER

The Visual apparatus which is such a dominating feature of the chick brain is a favorable structure to follow through the stages shown in figures 4 and 5. It is a good representative of the principle of early determination of the various regions of the neural tube. As early as the 11-somite


fig. 6 Outline drawing of centrzil nervous system of a 25~s0mite human embryo (Carnegie Collection, No. 6097). The somites and the condensed tissues of the branchial region are the only body parts that seem to be calling for help from the nervous system and the form of the neural tube is nicely fitted to their service. The character of segmentation shown by them is correspondingly reflected in the neural tube. Otherwise, the environment at this time offers the brain no inducements to segmentation. The endothelial apparatus is at first quite indi— pendent of the nervous system.


stage the eye vesicle begins to acquire a stalk—like constriction and at 17 somites one may speak of an optic foramen. This actually diminishes in size up to the 21—somite stage and then, due to growth of the structures in the adjoining wall, it is converted into the optic recess and becomes finally a mere slit. Of the other parts of the Visual apparatus the thalamus and optic lobe appear early. The older anatomists spoke of the ‘optic thalamus.’ In the embryo the relation between the thalamus and the optic evagination is very close, being contiguous at the 8—somite stage. This continuity is later expressed in the optic tract. The optic lobe as a terminal visual center undergoes an extraordinary growth. As is true for the cerebral hemisphere in man, it first establishes a great surface expansion. This mechanically favors the subsequent stratification and correlating fiber sheets which characterize the later thickening of the wall. In the 3- and 4-day chick the right and left optic lobes open widely into each other and give the appearance of being one optic lobe, which we all delight to call the mid-brain vesicle. In reality the optic lobes start as bilateral expansion of the neural wall and maintain their bilaterality throughout. This is conspicuously true in the chick where in adaptation to the available space the two lobes become separated like saddle-bags on the sides of the brain-stem, connection being maintained by a broad lamina of commissural fibers.


In studying the optic lobes one can see that they are distinct and can be demarcated ventrally from the basal plate. This basal plate is destined to become an important part of the brain. It becomes the main connecting trunk of the brainstem. It is the cerebral ‘main street’ or an elongated ‘market’ along Whose margins are the great nerve centers from which it receives and to which it distributes fibers. While its primary purpose appears to be concerned with traffic, various effecting, regulating, and correlating centers are interspersed along its course. From the point of View of this paper it is to be noted that it has an identity and that it is a longitudinal structure which is not interrupted by transverse partitions such as would be essential in a system of three brain vesicles. To draw a line across it as we have been accustomed to do in the past, saying what is in front of the line is one thing and what is behind it another, is an act of rank pedagogic violence. Anteriorly, the basal plate disappears below the thalamus and corpus striatum blending with the great fiber systems which come from them and course on their lateral surfaces. With its extension caudalward the basal plate undergoes enormous growth throughout the peduncular and pontine regions where it will be recognized as the formatio reticularis which in turn is continuous with the large ground bundles of the spinal cord. The term ‘peduncles of the brain’ has been granted by custom to specific bundles of fibers which have not reached the basal plate in the earlier stages. Otherwise one might wish to use the term ‘pedunculus cerebri’ instead of basal plate for this structure from the beginning. This, however, is not the place to discuss the terminology of this part of the adult brain for which there is so great an opportunity-»a region where we yet speak of a ‘tcctum’ of the ‘tegmentum.’


Between the optic lobe and the thalamus a new subdivision or focal area of active proliferation appears at the 21-somite stage, and this has been designated as the metathalamus abiding the time when a more specific designation can be arrived at. It appears to correspond to a group of pretectal nuclei.


A conspicuous feature of the early neural tube is a segmental-like effect which is associated with the innervation of the facial processes and the branchial bars. Already. at the 8- and 11—somite stages one can see expansions of the neural wall at foci of active proliferation producing transverse grooves and intervening ridges. With some variation between Vertebrate classes and orders, there is a general similarity in the existence of these grooves among all verte— bratcs. The grooves in the chick are not quite the same as those in pig and man, but as in these two orders they show a constant relationship to particular visceral cranial nerves. The points at which the respective nerves emerge from the neural wall and their relation to the grooves were plotted as carefully as possible and are shown for the successive stages in figures 4 and 5. It is to be noted that the different grooves possess certain individualities, in shape, width, length, and point of nerve exit. Furthermore, there are more grooves than nerves. The first two must be allotted to the trigeminal nerve and there is no nerve for the groove between the facial and glossopharyngeal nerves unless it be the acoustic nerve Which is not a visceral nerve. None of the grooves extend across the median line, even at their maximum develop ment at the end of the second day, being interrupted by the median raphe. The basal plate gradually spreads out ventral to them and as it increases in mass, being unsegmental itself, the grooves gradually disappear. These grooves are much more marked and appear to have no continuity with the type of segmentation that characterizes the neural tubes opposite the somites, which is quite contrary to the conclusion arrived at by Hill. Peripherally, the visceral cranial nerves correspond precisely to the condensed cell masses they innervate, as can be seen in figure 6, and it seems probable that their growth is a neural response to their particular environment. They would therefore be expected to exhibit the same type of segmentation prevailing there, which is a very different sort from that of the somites.

Summary

From morphological evidence one must conclude that the nearest approach to metamerism in the embryonic central nervous system is that occurring in the spinal region, and even this appears to be an adaptation to the Somites that lie alongside. The segmentation is evidently primary in the Somites, which during the early stages make an impress on the cord. ‘

A different type of segmentation is found in the branchial region——a type that is not continuous with and is different in histological character from the somite—type of the cord. The condensed cellular masses that are to form the tissues and musculature of the maxillary, mandibular, and throat regions receive appropriate and early innervation from a series of nerves, the visceral cranial nerves, which are clearly associated with a series of grooves in the neural Wall. These bilaterally placed transverse grooves are the expression of active cell proliferation and represent foci of origin with the associated nuclei, of these visceral cranial nerves. Although resembling each other in general, these grooves possess individualities which distinguish them from one another, and in the different vertebrate orders there are certain variations in their finer morphological details. The trigeminal and facial are the first to establish themselves.


A third category of ‘segmentation’ is brought about by expansions of the neural wall with intervening constrictions associated with the development of definite brain parts. These subdivisions give evidence of early determination of the more fundamental structures which can be followed through to their final form in the mature brain. From the outset they are characterized in each case by individuality of form and are not serial repetitions of a uniform pattern. Reexamination of similar material does not substantiate the well-known figure of Hill showing a neural tube, metameric from stem to stern. Instead of a rigid metameric system, the neural tube shows itself responsive at all levels to its environment. VVhere the environment is truly segmented the tube takes on that character in some degree. At levels where the environment is branchiomerie there we find the neural tube responding with suitable cranial nerves. At levels still further forward, aside for certain special-sense organs, there appear to be no environmental demands, at least in the early stages, and there the neural tube devotes itself to its own requirements in the way of centers of correlation and control, the subdivision of which bears no resemblance to true segmentation.


The subdivision of the embryonic brain into three primary brain vesicles is an arbitrary expedient rather than a natural phenomenon. The scheme is not to be blamed on His. 'When he and his associates on the Committee for revision of Anatomical Nomenclature utilized these divisions of the brain and their subdivisions, it was done only as an aid in regional localization. No such far-reaching interpretation was inferred as that of the metameric enthusiasts. There is a certain convenience in the terms fore-brain, mid-brain, and hindbrain, and if it is remembered that there never are three primary vesicles serially equivalent and precisely demarcated, corresponding to the three brain divisions, then no harm is done. If, however, the student is taught that the neural tube becomes dilated anteriorly into three equivalent homogeneous vesicles which are to become the brain, and that it is subsequent to the acquirement of their vesicular state that these become differentiated and subdivided into actual brain structures, the said student will have been taught something that he can never verify, and it is likely that his conception of organogenesis will lean more toward the principles of the tailor—shop than toward those typical of living embryonic tissue.


If intellectual stimulation is the object in view, how much more thrilling than the three—brain—vesicle lore is the knowledge which is now available concerning the potentialities of embryonic tissues and concerning the influence of the tissues upon each other. These interactions in their general principles are demonstrable facts and may be safely intrusted to the embryological armamentarium of the student. It is inconceivable that any rigid geometrical schema could be as marvelous as the reality of the blending adaptation maintained between the embryonic nervous system and the body structures which it is to serve, as they exist at the moment, whereby the two are continuously and perfectly coordinated. Thought of in these terms, the form of the neural tube and its modifications from stage to stage take on important significance.